Journal Information
Vol. 3. Issue 3.
Pages 101-109 (May - June 2007)
Share
Share
Download PDF
More article options
Vol. 3. Issue 3.
Pages 101-109 (May - June 2007)
Full text access
Cognition and Perception Deficits in Fibromyalgia and Rheumatoid Arthritis
Déficit mnésicos y perceptivos en la fibromialgia y la artritis reumatoide
Visits
5181
Lola Roldán-Tapia
Corresponding author
mdroldan@ual.es

Correspondence: Facultad de Humanidades y Ciencias de la Educación II. Universidad de Almería. Ctra. de Sacramento, s/n. 04120 La Cañada de San Urbano. Almería. España.
, Rosa Cánovas-López, José Cimadevilla, Matías Valverde
Departamento de Neurociencia y Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad de Almería, Almería, Spain
This item has received
Article information
Introduction

Cognitive disturbance in patients with fibromyalgia and rheumatoid arthritis is today a topic of a great clinical interest, largely due to the fact that these persons often complain about cognitive problems.

Objective

This study is aimed to assess the visuospatial memory, attention, and perceptive capacities in chronic pain patients.

Material and methods

Groups were constituted by fibromyalgia patients and rheumatoid arthritis patients, as well as a control group. All the subjects completed a battery of visual and spatial memory, speed of processing, working memory, attention, orientation, and visuoperceptive abilities. A cognitive reserve measurement was obtained.

Results

Results show that chronic pain patients displayed worse cognitive performance than controls. Moreover, arthritis patients execute poorly when compared to the group of fibromyalgia in tasks that demand visuoperceptive integration and visuomotor processing. Patients suffering fibromyalgia obtained worse punctuations than those with arthritis in spatial memory and spatial orientation tasks.

Conclusions

Both groups developed important cognitive deficits, which cannot be explained by the collateral effects of such pathologies, because cognitive profiles are not similar and appear from the beginning of the disease.

Key words:
Neuropsychologic alterations
Rheumatoid arthritis
Fibromyalgia
Memory
Neuropsychology
Cognitive reserve
Introducción

El deterioro cognitivo en los pacientes con dolor crónico es hoy día un tema de gran interés clínico, debido a que estas personas con frecuencia se quejan de problemas cognitivos.

Objetivo

Investigar las capacidades de atención, perceptivas y de memoria visuoespacial en los pacientes con dolor crónico en comparación con un grupo control y evaluar si entre estos pacientes hay diferencias, teniendo en cuenta las características de la propia enfermedad y la capacidad de reserva cognitiva.

Material y métodos

Se seleccionó a 2 grupos de pacientes, enfermos de fibromialgia y artritis reumatoide con corta duración de la enfermedad, y se los comparó con un grupo control. Todos los sujetos completaron un protocolo de exploración de memoria visual y espacial, velocidad de procesamiento, memoria de trabajo, visuopercepción, atención y orientación. Para todos ellos se obtuvo una medida de reserva cognitiva.

Resultados

Los pacientes con dolor crónico presentan una peor ejecución cognitiva que los controles. Los pacientes con artritis ejecutan la tarea peor que los de fibromialgia cuando requiere mayor velocidad de procesamiento visuomotor y un déficit en la integración en tareas visuoperceptivas. Los enfermos de fibromialgia obtuvieron peores puntuaciones en las tareas de memoria espacial y orientación.

Conclusiones

Los pacientes aquejados de fibromialgia y artritis tienen déficit aun cuando la cronicidad de la enfermedad sea breve. Dicho déficit parece no explicarse por efectos colaterales de dichas afecciones, ya que los perfiles cognitivos no son similares y aparecen desde el inicio de la enfermedad.

Palabras clave:
Alteraciones neuropsicológicas
Artritis reumatoide
Fibromialgia
Memoria
Neuropsicología
Reserva cognitiva
Full text is only aviable in PDF
References
[1.]
Sociedad Española de Reumatología.
Estudio EPISER: Prevalencia de las enfermedades reumáticas en la población española.
Merck, Sharp & Dohme, (2001),
[2.]
J.M. Glass, D.C. Park, M. Minear, L.J. Crofford.
Memory beliefs and function in fibromyalgia patients.
J Psychosom Res, 58 (2005), pp. 263-269
[3.]
S.E. Sephton, J.L. Studts, K. Hoover, I. Weissbecker, G. Lynch, I. Ho, et al.
Biological and psychological factors associated with memory fuction in fibromyalgia syndrome.
Health Psychol, 6 (2003), pp. 592-597
[4.]
B. Dick, C. Eccleston, G. Crombez.
Attentional functioning in fibromyalgia, rheumatoid arthritis and musculoskeletal pain patients.
Arthritis Rheum, 47 (2002), pp. 639-644
[5.]
M. Bartolini, M. Candela, M. Brugni, L. Catena, F. Mari, G. Pomponio, et al.
Are behaviour and motor performances of rheumatoid arthritis patients influenced by subclinical cognitive impairments? A clinical and neuroimaging study.
Clin Exp Rheumatol, 20 (2002), pp. 491-497
[6.]
D.C. Park, J.M. Glass, M. Minear, L.J. Crofford.
Cognitive function in fibromyalgia patients.
[7.]
J. Shur.
Neuropsychological impairment in fibromyalgia Relation to depression, fatigue and pain.
J Psychosom Res, 55 (2003), pp. 321-329
[8.]
Y. Stern.
The concept of cognitive reserve: A catalyst for research.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 25 (2003), pp. 589-593
[9.]
J.L. Sanchez, M. Rodriguez, J. Carro.
Influence of cognitive reserve on neuropsychologic functioning in Alzheimer's disease type sporadic in subject of spanish nationality.
Neuropsy Neuropsychol Behav Neurol, 15 (2002), pp. 113-122
[10.]
M.D. Lezak.
Neuropsychological assesment.
3.ª, Oxford University Press, (1995),
[11.]
C.J. Golden.
Test de Colores y Palabras (Stroop).
TEA, (1994),
[12.]
D. Wechsler.
Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para adultos.
TEA, (1993),
[13.]
A. Rey.
Test de copia y reproducción de memoria de figuras geométricas complejas.
TEA, (1997),
[14.]
A. Achiron, M. Polliack, S.M. Rao, Y. Barak, M. Lavie, N. Appelboin, et al.
Cognitive patterns and progression in multiple sclerosis: construction and validation of percentile curves.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 76 (2005), pp. 744-749
[15.]
D. Wechsler.
Escala de memoria de Wechsler-III (WMS-III).
TEA, (2004),
[16.]
A.L. Benton, A.B. Sivan, K.D. Hamsher, N.R. Varney, S. Otfried.
Contribution to Neuropsychological assesment.
2.ª, Oxford University Press, (1994),
[17.]
G.M. Grace, W.R. Nielson, M. Hopkins, M.A. Berg.
Concentration and memory deficits in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 4 (1999), pp. 477-487
[18.]
F. Leavitt, R.S. Katz.
Distraction as a key determinant of impaired memory in patients with fibromyalgia.
J Rheumatol, 33 (2006), pp. 127-132
[19.]
J. Grisart, M. van der Linden, E. Masquelier.
Controlled Processes and automaticity in memory functioning in fibromyalgia patients: Relation with emocional distress and hypervigilance.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 8 (2002), pp. 994-1009
[20.]
Y. Stern.
Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord, 20 (2006), pp. S69-S74
[21.]
N. Scarmeas, Y. Stern.
Cognitive reserve: implications for diagnosis and prevention of Alzheimer's disease.
Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep, 4 (2004), pp. 374-380
[22.]
A.S. Ciesielski, S. Samson, B.J. Steinhoff.
Neuropsychological and psychiatric impact of add-on titration of pregabalin versus levetiracetam: a comparative short-term study.
Epilepsy Behav, 9 (2006), pp. 424-431
[23.]
I. Hindmarch, L. Trick, F. Ridout.
A double-blind, placebo- and positiveinternal-controlled (alprazolam) investigation of the cognitive and psychomotor profile of pregabalin in healthy volunteers.
Psychopharmacology (Berl), 183 (2005), pp. 133-143
[24.]
E. Beghi.
Efficacy and tolerability of the new antiepileptic drugs: comparison of two recent guidelines.
Lancet Neurol, 3 (2004), pp. 618-621
[25.]
S.A. Montgomery.
Pregabalin for the treatment of generalised anxiety disorder.
Expert Opin Pharmacother, 7 (2006), pp. 2139-2154
[26.]
L.J. Crofford, M.C. Rowbotham, P.J. Mease, I.J. Russell, R.H. Dworkin, A.E. Corbin, Pregabalin 1008-105 Study Group, et al.
Pregabalin for the treatment of fibromyalgia syndrome: results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial.
Arthritis Rheum, 52 (2005), pp. 1264-1273
[27.]
C. Munizza, L. Olivieri, G. di Loreto, P. Dionisio.
A comparative, randomized, double-blind study of trazodone prolonged-release and sertraline in the treatment of major depressive disorder.
Curr Med Res Opin, 22 (2006), pp. 1703-1713
[28.]
G. Martinon-Torres, M. Fioravanti, E.J. Grimley.
Trazodone for agitation in dementia.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 18 (2004), pp. CD004990
[29.]
C. Lennkh, P. Fischer, B. Kufferle, S. Kasper.
Occurrence of trazodone-induced delirium.
Int Clin Psychopharmacol, 13 (1998), pp. 225-228
[30.]
P.H. Silverstone, J.E. Rue, M. Franklin, K. Hallis, G. Camplin, D. Laver, et al.
The effects of administration of mCPP on psychological, cognitive, cardiovascular, hormonal and MHPG measurements in human volunteers.
Int Clin Psychopharmacol, 9 (1994), pp. 173-178
[31.]
H. Knegtering, M. Eijck, A. Huijsman.
Effects of antidepressants on cognitive functioning of elderly patients. A review.
Drugs Aging, 5 (1994), pp. 192-199
[32.]
I. Amado-Boccara, N. Gougoulis, M.F. Poirier-Littre, A. Galinowski, H. Loo.
Effects of antidepressants on cognitive functions.
Review of the literature. Encephale, 20 (1994), pp. 65-77
[33.]
Martinón-Torres G, Fioravanti M, Grimley Evans J. Trazodona para la agitación en la demencia (Revisión Cochrane traducida). De La Biblioteca Cochrane Plus, número 4, 2006. Oxford: Update Software Ltd.
[34.]
J. Caballero, M. Hitchcock, D. Beversdorf, D. Scharre, M. Nahata.
Long term effects of antidepressants on cognition in patients with Alzheimer's disease.
J Clin Pharm Ther, 31 (2006), pp. 593-598
[35.]
S. Simis, R. Nitrini.
Cognitive improvement after treatment of depressive symptoms in the acute phase of stroke.
Arq Neuropsiquiatr, 64 (2006), pp. 412-417
[36.]
T.E. Wroolie, K.E. Williams, J. Keller, L.N. Zappert, S.D. Shelton, H.A. Kenna, et al.
Mood and neuropsychological changes in women with midlife depression treated with escitalopram.
J Clin Psychopharmacol, 26 (2006), pp. 361-366
[37.]
E.J. Rose, E. Simonotto, E.P. Spencer, K.P. Ebmeier.
The effects of escitalopram on working memory and brain activity in healthy adults during performance of the n-back task.
Psychopharmacology (Berl), 185 (2006), pp. 339-347
[38.]
N. Egashira, Y. Matsumoto, K. Mishima, K. Iwasaki, M. Fujioka, M. Matsushita, et al.
Low dose citalopram reverses memory impairment and electroconvulsive shock-induced immobilization.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 83 (2006), pp. 161-167
[39.]
J.I. Friedman, R. Ocampo, Z. Elbaz, M. Parrella, L. White, S. Bowler, et al.
The effect of citalopram adjunctive treatment added to atypical antipsychotic medications for cognitive performance in patients with schizophrenia.
J Clin Psychopharmacol, 25 (2005), pp. 237-242
[40.]
K.F. Ng, T.S. Yuen, V.M. Ng.
A comparison of postoperative cognitive function and pain relief with fentanyl or tramadol patient-controlled analgesia.
J Clin Anesth, 18 (2006), pp. 205-210
[41.]
E. Freye, J.V. Levy.
The effects of tramadol on pain relief, fast EEG-power spectrum and cognitive function in elderly patients with chronic osteo arthritis (OA).
Acute Pain, 8 (2006), pp. 55-61
[42.]
W.A. Katz.
Pharmacology and clinical experience with tramadol in osteoarthritis.
Drugs, 52 (1996), pp. 141-153
[43.]
F. Puga, H. Veiga, M. Cagy, K. McDowell, R. Piedade, P. Ribeiro.
Analysis of the influence of bromazepam on cognitive performance through the visual evoked potential (P300).
Arq Neuropsiquiatr, 63 (2005), pp. 228-234
[44.]
K. Schaffler, W. Klausnitzer.
Placebo-controlled study on acute and subchronic effects of buspirone vs bromazepam utilizing psychomotor and cognitive assessments in healthy volunteers.
Pharmacopsychiatry, 22 (1989), pp. 26-33
[45.]
R. Fontaine, P. Mercier, P. Beaudry, L. Annable, G. Chouinard.
Bromaze pam and lorazepam in generalized anxiety: a placebo-controlled study with measurement of drug plasma concentrations.
Acta Psychiatr Scand, 74 (1986), pp. 451-458
[46.]
S. Schuck, H. Allain, J.M. Gandon, A. Patat, V. Millet, F. le Coz.
Effect of bromazepam versus placebo on inhibition and waiting capacity in young women with traits of anxiety.
Fundam Clin Pharmacol, 12 (1998), pp. 463-467
[47.]
T.F. Munte, E. Gehde, S. Johannes, M. Seewald, H.J. Heinze.
Effects of alprazolam and bromazepam on visual search and verbal recognition memory in humans: a study with event-related brain potentials.
Neuropsychobiology, 34 (1996), pp. 49-56
[48.]
M. Bourin, J.L. Auget, M.C. Colombel, C. Larousse.
Effects of single oral doses of bromazepam, buspirone and clobazam on performance tasks and memory.
Neuropsychobiology, 22 (1989), pp. 141-145
[49.]
S. Drabant, J. Tomlo, M. Toth, E. Peterfai, I. Klebovich.
The cognitive effect of alprazolam in healthy volunteers.
Acta Pharm Hung, 76 (2006), pp. 25-31
[50.]
E.H. Uhlenhuth, V. Starcevic, C. Qualls, E.J. Antal, W. Matuzas, J.I. Javaid, et al.
Abrupt discontinuation of alprazolam and cognitive style in patients with panic disorder: early effects on mood, performance, and vital signs.
J Clin Psychopharmacol, 26 (2006), pp. 519-523
[51.]
N. Pomara, T.M. Facelle, A.E. Roth, L.M. Willoughby, D.J. Greenblatt, J.J. Sidtis.
Dose-dependent retrograde facilitation of verbal memory in healthy elderly after acute oral lorazepam administration.
Psychopharmacology (Berl), 185 (2006), pp. 487-494
[52.]
B. Tiplady, E. Bowness, L. Stien, G. Drummond.
Selective effects of clonidine and temazepam on attention and memory.
J Psychopharmacol, 19 (2005), pp. 259-265
[53.]
D. Beracochea.
Anterograde retrograde effects of benzodiazepines on memory.
Sci World J, 16 (2006), pp. 1460-1465
[54.]
A. Takiguchi, T. Masuoka, Y. Yamamoto, A. Mikami, C. Kamei.
Potentiation of ethanol in spatial memory deficits induced by some benzodiazepines.
J Pharmacol Sci, 101 (2006), pp. 325-328
[55.]
L.M. Reder, J.M. Oates, E.R. Thornton, J.J. Quinlan, A. Kaufer, J. Sauer.
Druginduced amnesia hurts recognition, but only for memories that can be unitized.
Psychol Sci, 17 (2006), pp. 562-567
[56.]
A.M. Nogueira, S. Pompeia, J.C. Galduroz, O.F. Bueno.
Effects of a benzodiazepine on free recall of semantically related words.
Hum Psychopharmacol, 21 (2006), pp. 327-336
[57.]
J. Fisher, E. Hirshman, T. Henthorn, J. Arndt, A. Passannante.
Midazolam amnesia and short-term/working memory processes.
Conscious Cogn, 15 (2006), pp. 54-63
[58.]
P. Maruff, J. Werth, B. Giordani, A.F. Caveney, D. Feltner, P.J. Snyder.
A statistical approach for classifying change in cognitive function in individuals following pharmacologic challenge: an example with alprazolam.
Psychopharmacology (Berl), 186 (2006), pp. 7-17
[59.]
M.D. Veltmeyer, C.R. Clark, A.C. McFarlane, K.L. Felmingham, R.A. Bryant, E. Gordon.
Integrative assessment of brain and cognitive function in posttraumatic stress disorder.
J Integr Neurosci, 4 (2005), pp. 145-159
[60.]
J.A. Boscarino.
Posttraumatic stress disorder and physical illness: results from clinical and epidemiologic studies.
Ann N Y Acad Sci, 1032 (2004),
[61.]
K.G. Raphael, M.N. Janal, S. Nayak.
Comorbidity of fibromyalgia and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in a community sample of women.
Pain Med, 5 (2004), pp. 33-41
[62.]
P. Roy-Byrne, W.R. Smith, J. Goldberg, N. Afari, D. Buchwald.
Post-traumatic stress disorder among patients with chronic pain and chronic fatigue.
Psychol Med, 34 (2004), pp. 363-368
Copyright © 2007. Sociedad Española de Reumatología and Colegio Mexicano de Reumatología
Download PDF
Idiomas
Reumatología Clínica (English Edition)
Article options
Tools
es en

¿Es usted profesional sanitario apto para prescribir o dispensar medicamentos?

Are you a health professional able to prescribe or dispense drugs?